Allport, D. A. (1977). On knowing the meaning of words we are unable to report: The effects of visual masking. In Dornic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 505–533). Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Anderson, M. C., Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410(6826), 366–369.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Anil, A. E., Kivircik, B. B., Batur, S., Kabakçi, E., Kitis, A., Güven, E., . . .Arkar, H. (2003). The Turkish version of the Auditory Consonant Trigram Test as a measure of working memory: A normative study. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17(2), 159–169.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115–116.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170–177.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Bannon, S., Gonsalvez, C. J., Croft, R. J., Boyce, P. M. (2002). Response inhibition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 110(2), 165–174.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Benoni, H., Tsal, Y. (2010). Where have we gone wrong? Perceptual load does not affect selective attention. Vision Research, 50(13), 1292–1298.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Berkman, E. T., Burklund, L., Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Inhibitory spillover: Intentional motor inhibition produces incidental limbic inhibition via right inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage, 47(2), 705–712.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Boot, W. R., Brockmole, J. R., Simons, D. J. (2005). Attention capture is modulated in dual-task situations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 662–668.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Brand-D’Abrescia, M., Lavie, N. (2008). Task coordination between and within sensory modalities: Effects on distraction. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(3), 508–515.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon Press.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. F., FitzGerald, P., Parkes, K. R. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 1–16.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10(1), 12–21.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Catarino, A., Küpper, C. S., Werner-Seidler, A., Dalgleish, T., Anderson, M. C. (2015). Failing to forget: Inhibitory-control deficits compromise memory suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Science, 26(5), 604–616.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N. A., Blackwell, A. D., Robbins, T. W., Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(7), 1282–1284.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Chen, Z., Cave, K. R. (2013). Perceptual load vs. dilution: The roles of attentional focus, stimulus category, and target predictability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 327.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Chen, Z., Cave, K. R. (2015). Singleton search is guided by knowledge of the target, but maybe it shouldn’t be. Vision Research, 115, 92–103.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
de Fockert, J. W. (2013). Beyond perceptual load and dilution: A review of the role of working memory in selective attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 287.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
de Fockert, J. W., Bremner, A. J. (2011). Release of inattentional blindness by high working memory load: Elucidating the relationship between working memory and selective attention. Cognition, 121(3), 400–408.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291(5509), 1803–1806.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Deutsch, J. A., Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70(1), 80–90.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Driver, J. (2001). A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. British Journal of Psychology, 92(1), 53–78.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Eriksen, B. A., Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Fenn, E., Blandón-Gitlin, I., Coons, J., Pineda, C., Echon, R. (2015). The inhibitory spillover effect: Controlling the bladder makes better liars. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 112–122.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Forster, S., Lavie, N. (2007). High perceptual load makes everybody equal. Psychological Science, 18(5), 377–381.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Frings, C., Schneider, K. K., Fox, E. (2015). The negative priming paradigm: An update and implications for selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1577–1597.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Gaspelin, N., Luck, S. J. (2018). The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 79–92.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Geng, J. J. (2014). Attentional mechanisms of distractor suppression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 147–153.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Gil-Gómez de Liaño, B., Stablum, F., Umiltà, C. (2016). Can concurrent memory load reduce distraction? A replication study and beyond. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), e1–e12.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Griffiths, D., Tadic, S. D. (2008). Bladder control, urgency, and urge incontinence: Evidence from functional brain imaging. Neurourology and Urodynamics, 27(6), 466–474.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Itthipuripat, S., Cha, K., Byers, A., Serences, J. T. (2017). Two different mechanisms support selective attention at different phases of training. PLoS Biology, 15(6), Article e2001724.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Johnston, W. A., Dark, V. J. (1982). In defense of intraperceptual theories of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(3), 407–421.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R. A., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(14), 8410–8413.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Karakaş, S., Erdoğan, E., Sak, L., Soysal, A. Ş., Ulusoy, T., Ulusoy, İ. Y., Alkan, S. (1999). Stroop Test TBAG Form: Standardisation for Turkish Culture, Reliability and Validity. Turkish Journal of Clinical Psyhiatry. 2(2), 75–88
Google Scholar
Konstantinou, N., Beal, E., King, J. R., Lavie, N. (2014). Working memory load and distraction: Dissociable effects of visual maintenance and cognitive control. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(7), 1985–1997.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Lappin, J. S., Eriksen, C. W. (1966). Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(6), 805–811.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451–468.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Lavie, N. (2000). Selective attention and cognitive control: Dissociating attentional functions through different types of load. In Monsell, S., Driver, J. (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 175–194). MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Lavie, N. (2001). Capacity limits in selective attention: Behavioral evidence and implications for neural activity. In Braun, J., Koch, C., Davis, J. L. (Eds.), Visual attention and cortical circuits (pp. 49–68). MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75–82.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143–148.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Lavie, N., Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8(5), 395–396.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Lavie, N., de Fockert, J. (2006). Frontal control of attentional capture in visual search. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 863–876.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339–354.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Lavie, N., Torralbo, A. (2010). Dilution: A theoretical burden or just load? A reply to Tsal and Benoni (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1657–1664.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Lavie, N., Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(2), 183–197.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Mayr, S., Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming as a memory phenomenon. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 215(1), 35–51.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
ISI
Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain Mapping, 12(3), 131–143.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Miller, J. (1987). Evidence of preliminary response preparation from a divided attention task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 425–434.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.
Comments (0)