Bach, P., Peatfield, N. A., Tipper, S. P. (2007). Focusing on body sites: The role of spatial attention in action perception. Experimental Brain Research, 178, 509–517.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Balaban, H., Assaf, D., Arad Meir, M., Luria, R. (2019). Different features of real-world objects are represented in a dependent manner in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication.
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000716 Google Scholar
Balaban, H., Assaf, D., Arad Meir, M., Luria, R. (2020). Different features of real-world objects are represented in a dependent manner in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(7), 1275–1293.
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000716 Google Scholar
Bastin, C., Besson, G., Simon, J., Delhaye, E., Geurten, M., Willems, S., Salmon, E. (2019). An integrative memory model of recollection and familiarity to understand memory deficits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, Article E281.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19000621 Google Scholar
Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., Alvarez, G. A., Oliva, A. (2013). Real-world objects are not represented as bound units: Independent forgetting of different object details from visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 791–808.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Buehner, M. J., Humphreys, G. R. (2009). Causal Contraction. Psychological Science, 21(1), 44–48.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609354735 Google Scholar
Carpenter, A. C., Schacter, D. L. (2017). Flexible retrieval: When true inferences produce false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 335–349.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Corbett, J. E. (2016). The whole warps the sum of its parts. Psychological Science, 28(1), 12–22.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616671524 Google Scholar
Coren, S., Girgus, J. S. (1980). Principles of perceptual organization and spatial distortion: The gestalt illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 404–412.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.6.3.404 Google Scholar
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694 Google Scholar
Gray, K. L., Barber, L., Murphy, J., Cook, R. (2017). Social interaction contexts bias the perceived expressions of interactants. Emotion, 17(4), 567–571.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Im, H. Y., Chong, S. C. (2014). Mean size as a unit of visual working memory. Perception, 43(7), 663–676.
https://doi.org/10.1068/p7719 Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 175–219.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Kaiser, D., Peelen, M. V. (2018). Transformation from independent to integrative coding of multi-object arrangements in human visual cortex. NeuroImage, 169, 334–341.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.065 Google Scholar
Kersten, A. W., Earles, J. L., Upshaw, C. (2013). False recollection of the role played by an actor in an event. Memory & Cognition, 41(8), 1144–1158.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0334-5 Google Scholar
Papeo, L., Stein, T., Soto-Faraco, S. (2017). The two-body inversion effect. Psychological Science, 28(3), 369–379.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616685769 Google Scholar
Quadflieg, S., Westmoreland, K. (2019). Making sense of other people’s encounters: Towards an integrative model of relational impression formation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43(2), 233–256.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00295-1 Google Scholar
Spachtholz, P., Kuhbandner, C. (2017). Visual long-term memory is not unitary: Flexible storage of visual information as features or objects as a function of affect. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17, 1141–1150.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Treisman, A. (2006). How the deployment of attention determines what we see. Visual Cognition, 14, 411–443.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Utochkin, I. S., Brady, T. F. (2020). Independent storage of different features of real-world objects in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149, 530–549.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Vestner, T., Gray, K. L. H., Cook, R. (2020). Why are social interactions found quickly in visual search tasks? Cognition, 200, Article 104270.
Google Scholar
Vestner, T., Gray, K. L. H., Cook, R. (2021). Visual search for facing and non-facing people: The effect of actor inversion. Cognition, 208, Article 104550.
Google Scholar
Vestner, T., Tipper, S. P., Hartley, T., Over, H., Rueschemeyer, S.-A. (2019). Bound together: Social binding leads to faster processing, spatial distortion, and enhanced memory of interacting partners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(7), 1251–1268.
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000545 Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Love, J., Selker, R., Gronau, Q. F., Šmíra, M., Epskamp, S., Matzke, D., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 35–57.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3 Google Scholar
Walbrin, J., Downing, P., Koldewyn, K. (2018). Neural responses to visually observed social interactions. Neuropsychologia, 112, 31–39.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.023 Google Scholar
Woodman, G. F., Vecera, S. P., Luck, S. J. (2003). Perceptual organization influences visual working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 80–87.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196470 Google Scholar
Comments (0)