Abel, S., Dressel, K., Bitzer, R., Kümmerer, D., Mader, I., Weiller, C., & Huber, W. (2009). The separation of processing stages in a lexical interference fMRI-paradigm. NeuroImage, 44, 1113–1124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.018
Abel, S., Dressel, K., Weiller, C., & Huber, W. (2012). Enhancement and suppression in a lexical interference fMRI-paradigm. Brain and Behavior: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective, 2, 109–127. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.31
Alario, F. X., Chainay, H., Lehericy, S., & Cohen, L. (2006). The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word production. Brain Research, 1076(1), 129–143.
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Anders, R., Llorens, A., Dubarry, A.-S., Trébuchon, A., Liégeois-Chauvel, C., & Alario, F.-X. (2019). Cortical dynamics of semantic priming and interference during word production: An intracerebral study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(7), 978–1001. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01406
Belke, E. (2008). Effects of working memory load on lexical-semantic encoding in language production. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 357–363. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.2.357
Belke, E., Meyer, A. S., & Damian, M. F. (2005). Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A Hum. Exp. Psychol., 58, 667–692. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000142
Belke, E., Shao, Z., & Meyer, A. S. (2017). Strategic origins of early semantic facilitation in the blocked-cyclic naming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 1659–1668. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000399
Berlingeri, M., Devoto, F., Gasparini, F., Saibene, A., Corchs, S. E., Clemente, L., Danelli, L., Gallucci, M., Borgoni, R., Borghese, N. A., & Paulesu, E. (2019). Clustering the brain with “CluB”: A new toolbox for quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging data. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.01037
Boylan, C., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2015). Compositionality and the angular gyrus: A multi-voxel similarity analysis of the semantic composition of nouns and verbs. Neuropsychologia, 78, 130–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.007
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Brett, M., Christoff, K., Cusack, R., & Lancaster, J. (2001). Using the Talairach atlas with the MNI template. NeuroImage, 13, 85.
Butterworth, B. (1989). Lexical access in speech production., in: Lexical representation and process. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, US, pp. 108–135.
Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Strijkers, K., Branzi, F. M., Costa, A., & Abutalebi, J. (2016). Semantic interference and its control: A functional neuroimaging and connectivity study. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 4179–4196. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23304
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 177–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432997381664
Cattaneo, Z., Pisoni, A., & Papagno, C. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation over Broca’s region improves phonemic and semantic fluency in healthy individuals. Neuroscience, 183, 64–70.
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Church, J. A., Balota, D. A., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2011). Manipulation of length and lexicality localizes the functional neuroanatomy of phonological processing in adult readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 1475–1493. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21515
Collins, A., & Loftus, E. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic memory. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.
Costa, A., & ALARIO, F.-X., Caramazza, A.,. (2005). On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture–word interference paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 125–131.
Costa, A., Strijkers, K., Martin, C., & Thierry, G. (2009). The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U. S. A., 106, 21442–21446. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908921106
Article CAS ADS Google Scholar
Damian, M. F., & Bowers, J. S. (2009). Assessing the role of orthography in speech perception and production: Evidence from picture-word interference tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 581–598. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440801896007
Damian, M. F., & Martin, R. C. (1999). Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 345–361. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.25.2.345
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Devlin, J. T., Matthews, P. M., & Rushworth, M. F. (2003). Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: A combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 71–84.
de Zubicaray, G., Fraser, D., Ramajoo, K., & McMahon, K. (2017). Interference from related actions in spoken word production: Behavioural and fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia, 96, 78–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.010
De Zubicaray, G., Johnson, K., Howard, D., & McMahon, K. (2014). A perfusion fMRI investigation of thematic and categorical context effects in the spoken production of object names. Cortex, 54, 135–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.018
de Zubicaray, G., McMahon, K., & Howard, D. (2015). Perfusion fMRI evidence for priming of shared feature-to-lexical connections during cumulative semantic interference in spoken word production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 261–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.848990
de Zubicaray, G. I., Hansen, S., & McMahon, K. L. (2013). Differential processing of thematic and categorical conceptual relations in spoken word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 131.
de Zubicaray, G. I., & Mcmahon, K. L. (2009). Auditory context effects in picture naming investigated with event-related fMRI. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 260–269. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.3.260
De Zubicaray, G. I., McMahon, K. L., Eastburn, M. M., & Wilson, S. J. (2002). Orthographic/phonological facilitation of naming responses in the picture-word task: An event-related fMRI study using overt vocal responding. NeuroImage, 16, 1084–1093. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1135
de Zubicaray, G.I., & Piai, V. (2019). Investigating the spatial and temporal components of speech production. Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistics, 471–497. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190672027.013.19
De Zubicaray, G. I., Wilson, S. J., McMahon, K. L., & Muthiah, S. (2001). The semantic interference effect in the picture-word paradigm: An event-related fMRI study employing overt responses. Human Brain Mapping, 14, 218–227. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.1054
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Dell, G. S., Burger, L. K., & Svec, W. R. (1997). Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model. Psychological Review, 104, 123–147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.123
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Devoto, F., Zapparoli, L., Bonandrini, R., Berlingeri, M., Ferrulli, A., Luzi, L., Banfi, G., & Paulesu, E. (2018). Hungry brains: A meta-analytical review of brain activation imaging studies on food perception and appetite in obese individuals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 94, 271–285.
Devoto, F., Zapparoli, L., Spinelli, G., Scotti, G., & Paulesu, E. (2020). How the harm of drugs and their availability affect brain reactions to drug cues: A meta-analysis of 64 neuroimaging activation studies. Translational Psychiatry, 10(1), 429.
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Diaz, M. T., Hogstrom, L. J., Zhuang, J., Voyvodic, J. T., Johnson, M. A., & Camblin, C. C. (2014). Written distractor words influence brain activity during overt picture naming. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00167
Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A. (2006). Lexical selection is not a competitive process: A reply to La Heij et al. (2006). Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70210-7
Garrett, M.F. (1980). Independent processing levels in sentence production. Fromkin (ed.) 263–271.
Gauvin, H. S., McMahon, K. L., & de Zubicaray, G. I. (2020). Top-down resolution of lexico-semantic competition in speech production and the role of the left inferior frontal gyrus: An fMRI study. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci., 36, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2020.1762904
Gentili, C., Cecchetti, L., Handjaras, G., Lettieri, G., & Cristea, I. A. (2021). The case for preregistering all region of interest (ROI) analyses in neuroimaging research. European Journal of Neuroscience, 53, 357–361. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14954
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Gentili, C., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Lettieri, G., Costa, C., & Cecchetti, L. (2019). ROI and phobias: The effect of ROI approach on an ALE meta-analysis of specific phobias. Human Brain Mapping, 40(6), 1814–1828. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24492
Hartwigsen, G., Weigel, A., Schuschan, P., Siebner, H. R., Weise, D., Classen, J., & Saur, D. (2016). Dissociating parieto-frontal networks for phonological and semantic word decisions: A condition-and-perturb TMS study. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 2590–2601. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv092
Henson, R. N. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in Neurobiology, 70(1), 53–81.
Comments (0)