What philosophy, if any, is needed for cognitive science?

ARMSTRONG, S., GLEITMAN, L., GLEITMAN, N. (1983). What some concepts might not be. In: «Cognition», vol. XIII, n. 3, pp. 263-308.

BARSALOU, L.W. (1982). Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. In: «Memory and Cognition», vol. X, n. 1, pp. 82-93.

BARSALOU, L.W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. In: «Memory and Cognition», vol. XI, n. 3, pp. 211-227.

BARSALOU, L.W. (1987). The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In: U. NEISSER (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 101-140.

BROOKS, R.A. (1991). Intelligence without representation. In: «Artificial Intelligence», vol. XLVII, n. 1-3, pp. 139-159.

BROOKS, R.A. (2002). Flesh and machines, Vintage, New York.

BRUNER, J.S., GOODNOW, J.J., AUSTIN, G.A. (1956). A study of thinking, Wiley, New York.

CHALMERS, D. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. In: «Journal of Consciousness Studies», vol. II, n. 3, pp. 200-219.

CHALMERS, D. (1996). The conscious mind, Oxford University Press, Oxford/New York.

CHURCHLAND, P.M. (1995). The engine of reason the seat of the soul, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

DREYFUS, H. (1979). What computers can’t do (1972) Harper & Row, New York.

DREYFUS, H. (1992). What computers still can’t do, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

FODOR, J.A. (1973). The language of thought, MIT Pres, Cambridge (MA).

FODOR, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

FODOR, J.A. (2008). LOT 2, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

GARCIA, J., KOELING, R.A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. In: «Psychonomic Science», vol. IV, n. 3, pp. 121-124.

GOODMAN, N. (1968). Languages of art, Bobbs and Merrill, New York.

GOODMAN, N. (1973). Problems and projects, Bobbs and Merrill, New York.

GOODMAN, N. (1978). Ways of worldmaking, Bobbs and Merrill, New York.

HARRIS, S., SCHETH, S.A., COHEN, M.S. (2008). Functional neuroimaging of belief, disbelief and uncertainty. In: «Annals of Neurology», vol. LXIII, pp. 141-147.

JACKSON, F. (1986). What Mary didn't know. In: «The Journal of Philosophy», vol. LXXXIII, n. 5, pp. 291-295.

KAHNEMAN, D. (1981). Who shall be the arbiter of our intuitions?. In: «Behavioral and Brain Sciences», vol. IV, n. 3, pp. 339-340.

KAHNEMAN, D., TVERSKY, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. In: «Cognitive Psychology», vol. III, n. 3, pp. 430-454.

KAHNEMAN, D., TVERSKY, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. In: «Psychological Review», vol. LXXX, n. 4, pp. 237-251.

KOSSLYN, S.M. (1980). Image and mind, Harvard University Press, Cambridge (MA).

KOSSLYN, S.M., BALL, J.M., REISER, B.J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information. In: «Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception», vol. IV, n. 1, pp. 47-60.

LEVINE, J. (1983). Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. In: «Pacific Philosophical Quarterly», vol. LXIV, n. 4, 1983, pp. 354-361.

LEVINE, J. (2001). Purple haze: The puzzle of consciousness, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

LIBET, B. (1965). Cortical activation in conscious and unconscious experience. In: «Perspectives in Biology and Medicine», vol. IX, n. 1, pp. 77-86.

LIBET, B. (2004). Mind time, Harvard University Press, Cambridge (MA).

LOFTUS, E. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. In: «Cognitive Psychology», vol. VII, n. 4, pp. 560-572.

LOGOTHETIS, N., SCHALL, J.D. (1989). Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. In: «Science», vol. CCXLV, n. 4919, pp. 761-763.

MACINTOSH, N.J. (1983). Conditioning and associative learning, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

MARKMAN, A.B. (1999). Knowledge representation, LEA, London.

MARR, D. (1982). Vision, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

MCCOON, R., RATCLIFF, G. (1979). Priming in episodic and semantic memory. In: «Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior», vol. XVIII, n. 4, pp. 463-480.

MCCOON, R., RATCLIFF, G. (1992). Spreading activation versus compound cue accounts of priming. In: «Journal of Experimental Psychology – Learning, Memory, and Cognition», vol. XVIII, n. 6, pp. 1155-1172.

MEDIN, D.L., SHAFFER, M.M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. In: «Psychological Review», vol. LXXXV, n. 3, pp. 207-238.

MINSKY, M. (1987). The society of mind, Touchstone, New York.

NAGEL, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat?. In: «Philosophical Review», vol. LXXXIII, n. 4, pp. 435-450.

OAKSFORD, M., CHATER, N. (2007). Bayesian rationality, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

OCCELLI, V. (2014). Molyneux’s question: A window on cross-modal interplay in blindness. In: «Rivista internazionale di Filosofia e di Psicologia», vol. V, n. 1, pp. 72-101.

POSNER, M.I., KEELE, S.W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. In: «Journal of Experimental Psychology», vol. LXXVII, n. 3, pp. 353-363.

PYLYSHYN, Z.W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells the minds brain. In: «Psychological Bulletin», vol. LXXX, n. 1, pp. 1-24.

PYLYSHYN, Z.W. (1978). Imagery and artificial intelligence. In: C. SAVAGE (ed.), Perception and cognition, Minnesota University Press, Minneapolis, pp. 19-55.

PYLYSHYN, Z.W. (1981). The imagery debate. In: «Psychological Review», vol. LXXXVIII, n. 1, pp. 16-45.

QUINE, W.V.O. (1960). Word and object, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

QUINE, W.V.O. (1969). Ontological relativity and other essays, Columbia University Press, New York.

QUINE, W.V.O. (1974). The roots of reference, Open Court, La Salle.

REINVILLE, P., DUNCAN, G.M., PRICE, D.D., CARRIER, B., BUSHNELL, M.C. (1997). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. In: «Science», vol. CCLXXVII, n. 5328, pp. 968-971.

RESCORLA, R.A., SOLOMON, R.L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian and instrumental learning. In: «Psychological Review», vol. LXXIV, n. 3, pp. 151-182.

ROSCH, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In: E. ROSCH, B.B. LLOYD (eds.), Cognition and categorization, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale (NJ), pp. 27-48.

RUSSELL, B. (1959). My philosophical development, Allen and Unwinn, London.

RYLE, G. (1949). The concept of mind, Hutchinson House, London.

SEARLE, J. (1980). Minds, brains and programs. In: «Behavioral and Brain Sciences», vol. III, n. 3, pp. 417-424.

SEARLE, J. (1990). Could a machine think?. In: «Scientific American», vol. CCLXII, n. 1, pp. 20-25.

SEARLE, J. (2002). Consciousness and language, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

SHEPARD, R.N. (1968). The mental image. In: «American Psychologist», vol. XXXIII, n. 2, pp. 125-137.

SHEPARD, R.N., COOPER, L.A. (1986). Mental images and their transformations, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

SKINNER, B.F. (1963). Behaviorism at fifty. In: «Science», vol. CXL, n. 3570, pp. 951-958.

SMITH CHURCHLAND, P. (1986). Neurophilosophy, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

SMITH CHURCHLAND, P. (2002). Brain-wise, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).

SMITH CHURCHLAND, P., CHURCHLAND, P.M. (1990). Could a machine think?. In: «Scientific American», vol. CCLXII, n. 1, pp. 26-31.

SOWA, J.F. (2000). Knowledge representation: Logical, philosophical and computational foundations, Brooks and Cole, Boston.

TENGLAS, E., VUL, E., GIROTTO, V., GONZALEZ, M., TENEBAUM, J.B., BONATTI, L.L. (2011). Pure reasoning in 12-month-old infants as probabilistic inference. In: «Science», vol. CCCXXXII, n. 6033, pp. 1054-1059.

TOLMAN, E. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. In: «Psychological Review», vol. LV, n. 4, pp. 189-208.

TURING, A.M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. In: «Mind», vol. LIX, n. 236, pp. 433-460.

WITTGENSTEIN, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations, Routledge, London.

Comments (0)

No login
gif