There is increasing recognition that gender norms affect adolescent health and well-being. This study explores the consistency of adolescents' gender norm perceptions across different dimensions (roles, traits, relations) and describes how the patterns of these perceptions vary across four culturally different settings.
MethodsThe study includes 8,977 adolescents aged 10–14 years from Kinshasa, Shanghai, Cuenca, and Indonesia. Three gender norm scales were examined: sexual double standard, gender stereotypical traits, and stereotypical roles. We investigated patterns of gender norms across dimensions (roles, traits, and relations) and compared results between sites. We also examined how adolescents' individual responses across the scales compared with average responses in their site, to assess the consistency of their gender views.
ResultsPatterns of gender norms varied across sites, reflected in different levels of endorsement of each gender norms scale, from least equal in Kinshasa to most equal in Shanghai, while greater variation of perspectives across gender dimensions was noted in Cuenca and Indonesia. Moving from a societal to an individual perspective, most adolescents in each site (62%–67%) held both more progressive and less progressive views compared with their average peer depending on the gender dimension.
ConclusionsOur study demonstrates the coexistence of multiple gender worldviews that are assessed and enacted as per adolescents' experiences and social context. Accounting for such complexities is essential for gender-transformative programs, as shifting gender attitudes in one area does not necessarily translate in more gender equitable views across other spheres of life.
KeywordsImplications and ContributionAdolescents' gender can vary widely across cultural contexts. Accounting for such complexities is essential for gender-transformative programs, as shifting gender attitudes in one area does not necessarily translate in more gender equitable views across other spheres of life.
Gender equality, a target of the Sustainable Development Goals, is a value in itself and a means to protect and promote human rights and human development [[1]United Nations Economic CouncilFrom the original sample of 9,990 adolescents surveyed across the four settings, 8,977 were retained in the current analysis after excluding 1,013 cases (10%) with missing data on any of the 17 gender norm questions that contribute to our primary outcomes. The final analytic sample comprised 2,767 adolescents in Kinshasa, 1,657 in Shanghai, 618 in Cuenca, and 3,935 in Indonesia, each corresponding to 97.4% of the original sample size in Kinshasa, 94.1% in Shanghai, 87.8% in Cuenca, and 84.0% in Indonesia. Sensitivity analysis comparing results among respondents of Cuenca and Indonesia included in the analysis and those excluded indicated that excluded individuals had slightly less stereotypical views than participants who provided complete answers.
Data collection took place in 2017 and 2018 and involved a two-hour long survey (including several breaks to reduce participant fatigue) collecting information on a range of topics including young people's family background and social circumstances, adverse child events, as well as their physical and mental health and well-being. The survey also included a series of questions assessing adolescents' perceptions of gender norms regulating romantic relationships and norms related to gender stereotypical traits and roles. We refer to gender stereotypes as reflecting conservative views about gender. The GEAS survey instrument is available at https://www.geastudy.org. The gender norm instrument is shown in WebAppendix. Measures Gender norm instrumentSeventeen questions scored on a five-point Likert scale ranging from “disagree a lot” to “agree a lot” were used to assess three dimensions of gender norm perceptions. A description of each survey item and the psychometric properties of each of the three gender scales are provided in Appendix 1: the sexual double standard (SDS), the gender stereotypical traits (GST), and the gender stereotypical roles (GSR). We refer to these measures as perceptions of gender norms, rather than attitudes or norms, as the questions relate to young people's perceptions about adolescents in their communities. In the absence of validated cross-cultural measures of gender norms for early adolescents, our first two scales were derived from GEAS formative work, grounded in the voices of young people and their parents in 14 sites across five continents [[26]Moreau C. Li M. De Meyer S. et al.Measuring gender norms about relationships in early adolescence: Results from the global early adolescent study.]. Distinct from the prior two scales, the GSR measure was derived from existing measures [[27]Measuring attitudes toward gender norms among young men in Brazil: Development and psychometric evaluation of the GEM scale.] to allow for comparisons and adaptation of our perceptions of gender norm measures as adolescents grow older across the longitudinal phase. In each setting, individual responses were combined to provide a mean score for each subscale, ranging from 1 to 5, with higher scores signaling perceptions of more unequal gender norms. Composite indicator of perceptions of gender inequalityWe derived a combined three-category measure of young people's perceptions of gender inequality based on the three aforementioned gender scales (SDS, GST, GSR). Because the scales were not normally distributed across all sites (all three scales were skewed in Kinshasa), we calculated the median score of each subscale in each site and classified respondents as per whether they scored greater or lower than the median for each scale in their own site. Participants who fell below the median score (which represent the average response of their peers) across the three gender norms scales were considered as having less gender unequal perceptions compared with their peers (type I: equal) while those who scored greater than the median across the three dimensions were considered having more gender unequal perceptions compared with their peers (type III: unequal). Respondents whose scores varied greater and lower than the median for the different scales were classified in an intermediate gender norms inequality category (type II: intermediate).
AnalysisWe first examined the basic sociodemographic composition of each sample. Next, we verified the psychometric properties of each scale (SDS, GST, GSR) (Appendix 1) and described the distribution of each gender norm scale in each site. Each scale was considered as a continuous measure ranging from one to 5 with higher scores reflecting perceptions of more unequal gender norms.Moving from a population to an individual level perspective, we examined the consistency in which adolescents had more gender equal views or more unequal gender views across multiple areas of life, by examining their individual gender norm perceptions across the three domains in comparison with their average peers. We used our composite measure described previously to estimate the percentage of individuals who systematically scored lower than their site median across the three gender norms dimensions (type I: equal), the percentage who consistently scored greater than their site median across all dimensions (type III: unequal), and the percentage who crossed the median depending on the dimension considered (type II: intermediate). We computed the mean score of each gender scale as per this typology to assess the differences in gender norm perceptions among adolescents' who fall at both ends of the gender equality spectrum in each site.
ResultsThe description of the study sample is shown in Table 1. The mean age ranged from 11.92 years ± 1.38 (standard deviation) in Kinshasa to 12.46 years ± .96 in Shanghai. Most adolescents lived with two parents although the family structure differed by the setting, with a greater proportion of adolescents living in single-parent households or with no parent in Kinshasa than other sites. Most students were enrolled in age appropriate school grades, although again in Kinshasa a higher percentage of adolescents were old for grade (37.0%) and 28.8% were out of school (as Kinshasa included an out-of-school sample). Literacy rates also varied widely ranging from 71.7% in Kinshasa to 99.5% in Cuenca.Table 1Sociodemographics of study population by site
At the population level, exploring the different dimensions of gender norms as perceived by adolescents, we found that mean gender norms scores varied substantially by site and gender domain (Figure 1A–C). Scores were highest in Kinshasa, indicative of more unequal norms and lower in Cuenca and Shanghai signaling greater perceptions of gender equality. Perceptions of GST were generally highest, followed by GSR, although in Cuenca, the highest mean score was observed for the SDS scale, and in Kinshasa, the mean GST score was slightly higher than the GSR mean score. The range in the scores also varied substantially in each site, suggesting substantial differences between individuals in these settings.Figure 1(A–C) Distribution of mean score of each gender norms scale per site.
Correlations between the three scales were generally low in all settings, with the Pearson correlation coefficients between SDS and GSR ranging from .05 in Cuenca and Shanghai to .17 in Kinshasa, which suggest a distinct nature of these different gender norm domains and complex patterns of gender norms across the sites. Correlations between SDS and GST varied from .15 in Cuenca to .36 in Kinshasa, while correlations between the GST and GSR varied from .27 in Kinshasa to .40 in Shanghai (Table 2).Table 2Site-specific correlations between three gender norm scales
GSR = gender stereotypical roles; GST = gender stereotypical traits; SDS = sexual double standard.
Turning to the assessment of adolescents' individual perceptions compared with their average peers, we found that two thirds of adolescents varied in their gender norm perceptions, holding more equal gender views than their average peer on some gender domains but less equal views on others (Figure 2). Between 13.7% and 16.6% of adolescents consistently scored lower than their average peer on all three gender norms scales and conversely between 16.2% and 21.8% consistently scored higher than their average peer on all three scales.Figure 2Distribution of theoretical composite measure of gender inequality/site. Type I (equal) indicates that the respondent has more equal gender perceptions than his/her site-specific average peer across all three gender domains (SDS, GST, GSR), type II (intermediate) indicates the respondent has both more equal and more unequal gender perceptions than his/her site-specific average peer depending on gender domains (SDS, GST, GSR). Type III (unequal) indicates that the respondent has more unequal gender perceptions than his/her site-specific average peer across all three gender domains (SDS, GST, GSR). GSR = gender stereotypical roles; GST = gender stereotypical traits; SDS = sexual double standard.
The gradient of gender egalitarian perceptions reflected in this typology is made evident in the increasing mean score of each gender scale (SDS, GST, GSR) indicative of more unequal gender perceptions moving across the typology, from type I representing greater gender egalitarian views to type III representing more inegalitarian gender views (Table 3).Table 3Mean scores for each scale based on composite measure categories
Gender scores range from 1 to 5, with higher scores signaling greater inequality.
GSR = gender stereotypical roles; GST = gender stereotypical traits; SDS = sexual double standard.
DiscussionThis cross-site study draws a complex picture of gender norm perceptions in early adolescence, especially because it provides empirical evidence of the extent to which norms vary among geographical settings, between individuals and within individuals. This finding confirms one important assumption of the gender norm theory: the gender system is ubiquitous but manifests itself in different ways as per time, place, or situation [[28]One egalitarianism or several? Two Decades of gender-role attitude change in Europe.].That patterns of gender norms vary across sites was reflected in the different levels of endorsement of each gender norms scale, as well as in the differences in the patterns of gender normative views across study settings. In general, adolescents' gender perceptions were least equal in Kinshasa and most equal in Shanghai, mirroring the distribution of the United Nations' Gender Inequality Index. The Gender Inequality Index indicates, on a scale of inequality from 0 to 1, that inequality is highest at .655 in the DRC, dropping to .451 and .389 in Indonesia and Ecuador, respectively, and lower at .163 in China (http://hdr.undp.org/en/data). These general patterns, however, conceal substantial variation in adolescents' perceptions of gendered traits and roles. Adolescents were more likely to endorse stereotypical traits, recognizing male strength over female vulnerabilities (a belief reported in a number of other studies around the globe [[12]Kågesten A. Gibbs S. Blum R. et al.Understanding factors that Shape gender attitudes in early adolescence globally: A Mixed-Methods systematic review.]), than stereotypical roles in family decisions and responsibilities (with the exception of Indonesia). Variation across gender dimensions was particularly evident in Ecuador and Indonesia (average scores varied from 2.24 to 3.61 and from 2.64 to 3.87, respectively), two countries sitting in the midrange of the United Nations gender inequality scale but both experiencing significant trends toward more equality since 1995 (http://hdr.undp.org/en/data). These results on the variation of perspectives
Comments (0)